Custom Search

Our Products · On-Line Store

Operational Aerospace Physiology *

Hypoxia

Hyperventilation

Trapped Gas Expansion

Decompression Sickness

G-Induced Loss of Consciousness (GLOC)

Spatial Disorientation

Loss of Situational Awareness

 

A Primer for the non-Aeromedical Professional

Aviation Physiology  

Introduction

Man's basic biological systems were designed to function at 1 ATA, on what is basically a two dimensional coordinate system, in which the only acceleration vectors are perpendicular to the ground.  When moving  the human to the reduced pressure, three dimensional arena of aviation, with its associated complex acceleration vectors - often greatly increased in magnitude - the basic physiological systems are pushed to, and sometimes beyond their limits of functionality.  It is the purpose of this section to briefly outline some of these concerns and what they mean to both the aircrew and to the clinician.

Hypoxia

"Hypoxia" has many subtleties of meanings to the medical community, depending on the area of specialty.  This discussion will be limited the applications that are of particular concern to the operational aviation community.  Therefore, in this light, hypoxia is defined as "an insufficient amount of oxygen delivered at the tissue level" - with the caveat that oxygen supply to the brain is the primary concern.  Four different "types" of hypoxia, categorized by the mechanism by which they disrupt oxygen supply to the brain, are identified and briefed to aircrew:  Hypoxic (or Altitude), Anemic (or Hypemic), Stagnant, and Histotoxic.

Hypoxic Hypoxia

This type of hypoxia can best be defined as "a deficiency in alveolar oxygen exchange", resulting primarily from the reduced partial pressure of oxygen as one goes to altitude.  At sea level, the alveolar partial pressure of oxygen (PAO2) is approximately 100 mmHg.  On the standard oxyhemoglobin dissociation curve, this translates to approximately 98% arterial oxyhemoglobin saturation.  Through research, healthy aircrew are found to be able to tolerate saturations as low as 87% without observable reduction in performance.  Again, using the dissociation curve, this can be found to occur when the PAO2  equals 60 mmHg, which occurs at 10,000 ft of altitude.  Because of this, there is a general oxygen use rule that states that supplemental oxygen must be used by all aircrew when flying at a cabin altitude exceeding 10,000 ft.  This rule, as well as exceptions/modifications to can be found in Chapter 8 of OPNAVINST 3710.7 (General Naval Aviation Training and Operating Procedures Standardization [NATOPS]).

As the altitude is increased above 10,000 feet, marked decrease in performance can be observed, with the endpoint being loss of consciousness and eventual death.  A measuring tool used in evaluation of hypoxia is the Time of Useful Consciousness (TUC), which is defined as the amount of time that an aircrew member has to recognize his hypoxic condition and still be capable of self-treatment.  The table below identifies approximate TUCs for several benchmark altitudes:

Altitude (ft)

Time of Useful Consciousness (TUC)

Up to 10,000 

Unlimited

18,000 

30-45 min

 25,000

3-5 min

35,000

30-60 sec

 43,000

10-15 sec

It should be noted that all of these are ranges, and that these represent population averages; clearly a given individual may fall outside of these guidelines.  The greatest significance of these figures is the non-linear rate in which the times decrease.  Any number of additional factors, both medical and environmental, could alter (typically shorten) these times.  Some of these factors will be discussed later in this article.

Prior to reaching the TUC, a number of recognizable symptoms may be present.  The most commonly reported symptoms (in no particular order) are: light-headedness, dizziness, numbness, tingling, air hunger, hot/cold flashes, fatigue, nausea, visual disturbances, euphoria, cyanosis, loss of coordination, hyperventilation, and confusion.  Actual symptoms experienced vary widely from individual to individual, even when exposed to identical environmental conditions.  All aircrew are trained in an altitude chamber, where hypoxia is induced at a simulated altitude of 25,000 ft; typically aircrew will be familiar with the symptoms, and will frequently self-diagnose hypoxia.  As virtually all of the above symptoms are perishable - they will resolve upon introduction of oxygen and/or return to lower altitudes - the land/ship-based clinician will have to assess the patient based on this self-diagnosis.

Anemic (Hypemic) Hypoxia

Although the terms have subtly different meanings "anemic" and "hypemic" are used fairly interchangeably in the arena of aviation physiology.  The Navy has historically embraced the term "anemic", whereas the USAF more typically uses "hypemic".  Only the former will be used throughout the remainder of this article.

Anemic Hypoxia can best be described as "a reduction in the oxygen carrying capacity of the blood" due either to physical or biochemical problems. Physically, the concern is blood loss, whether from wounds or from voluntary blood donation.  As blood loss and combat wound management are discussed elsewhere, those topics will not be re-addressed in this article.  Blood donation will be mentioned, as there is specific guidance to the aircrew in the General NATOPS (cited above).  Aircrew members are grounded for a period of 4 weeks following the donation of a unit of blood if flying in actual combat or in the shipboard environment.  If flying non-combat, shore-based operations, the grounding period is reduced to 4 days.  These periods are for the obvious circulatory re-hydration and  RBC production/maturation processes.  This grounding period is considered "self clearing" in that an "up-chit" from a flight surgeon is not required prior to return to flight status.  Because of these rules, the aircrew community should not be considered a viable source for blood bank replenishment.

The primary biochemical concern of anemic hypoxia is that of CO interference in the oxyhemoglobin formation process.  The natural 200x greater chemical affinity of hemoglobin to CO than to oxygen results in the formation of carboxyhemoglobin, with the resulting displacement of oxygen on the hemoglobin,  leading to a reduced amount of oxygen being transported to the tissues when CO is present.  In the aviation environment the greatest sources of CO are from exhaust and from smoking.  As per NATOPS, smoking is prohibited aboard all Naval aircraft, although there are no pre-flight restrictions.  Short of reaching clinical "CO poisoning" levels, symptoms linked to this type of hypoxia are fairly limited to headaches (mostly from exhaust inhalation) and a possible lowering in tolerance to other types of hypoxia.

Stagnant Hypoxia

Stagnant hypoxia is best described as "a reduction in the blood flow to the tissues", again with an emphasis on blood flow to the brain.  Because of the vertical nature of the human anatomy, the primary mechanical cause of this is the effect of the gravity vector, either normal or of increased magnitude due to flight profiles, on the heart-to-brain blood column.

One aspect of stagnant hypoxia is non-specific to aviation and is based on simple circulatory system mechanics.  When an individual is sitting in one position for an extended period of time, blood return from the legs can be compromised due to the lack of the muscle pump effect.  When this occurs, blood will tend to pool in the lower extremities, reducing the amount that return to the pulmonary system for re-oxygenation.  The resultant is a systemic lowering of oxygen levels and a slight lowering of cerebral blood pressure.  Symptomatically, this results in headaches, loss of attention, drowsiness, and ultimately falling asleep.  Walking around is considered the best "treatment" for this condition, and when that is not possible due to aircraft constraints, isometric contractions of the extremities, especially the legs, is recommended.

The other, more significant, aspect of stagnant hypoxia occurs when the magnitude of the gravity vector is increased through flight operations (typically referred to as "Pulling Gs").  This has been identified as one of the most significant aeromedical threats to the tactical jet aircrew member, and as such, will be discussed in a separate section of this article.

Histotoxic Hypoxia

Histotoxic hypoxia is the reduction in the ability of tissues - in particular the brain - to use oxygen, and is exclusively biochemical in nature.  As its name implies, the cause of this type of hypoxia is the presence of various toxins in the system that impact  the oxygen metabolism of the brain.  The two chemicals in particular that are discussed with aircrew are cyanide and ETOH.  Cyanide can be released during the combustion of some plastics and insulators used in aircraft construction; if there is an in-flight fire, cyanide poisoning must be considered as a possibility.  Of much greater importance is the potential impact of alcohol on the aircrew member.  In both cases, the toxin effects the cytochrome oxidase system, specifically cytochrome a3.  Although  actual intoxication at the time of flight is virtually unheard of, the residual (hangover) effects of alcohol in aircrew members is a real operational concern.  NATOPs states that consumption of alcohol is prohibited 12 hours prior to flight planning.  The rules go on to say that aircrew must be free of hangover effects.  Where the first half of this rule is easily quantifiable, the latter portion is very subjective and open to wide interpretation.  Although anecdotal observation and conversation indicates a fairly wide-spread disregard for these rules, especially the latter, there are no empirical studies to support these observations.   Symptoms of this type of hypoxia are not aviation-specific and are similar to any clinical presentation of cyanide exposure or alcohol usage.

Predisposing Factors and Cumulative Effects of Hypoxia

Although discussed above separately, it is quite possible for multiple types of hypoxia to be in effect simultaneously.  Some sources refer to the cumulative effects as being additive, others multiplicative, but which ever the case, the combined effect may exceed the sum of the individual parts.  As an example of this, consider a transport crew member who is a heavy smoker flying at 15,000 ft, slightly hung over, and near the end of a 4 hour mission.  At this point in time, the crew member is suffering from some anemic (residual CO) stagnant (blood pooling) and histotoxic (alcohol) effects.  If the aircraft were to lose its cabin pressure at this time, hypoxic hypoxia would be added to the mix.  Although each of the four aspects results in only a slight degradation in ability, the combination of them will result in an aircrew member whose performance will be far less than 100%.

Likewise, there are a number of factors which can predispose an individual to hypoxia, in that they may become symptomatic more quickly or develop more severe symptoms.  Among these are: Acclimatization (people living at higher altitudes have a better tolerance), Physical Activity (more activity uses more oxygen), Decompression Rate (TUCs can be reduce by 1/3 to 1/2 if the decompression is rapid/explosive), and Dehydration (blood circulation impairment).

Prevention and Treatment of Hypoxia

 To a large extent, prevention of hypoxia is common sense.  Maintain cabin pressure and avoid the various self-imposed stressors (smoking, alcohol, medications, etc) which can lead to the various types of hypoxia.  There are special procedures for G-induced stagnant hypoxia that will be discussed in a later section.  The aircrew are taught a basic five-step procedure to treat hypoxia if it is suspected: (1) 100% oxygen, (2) Check oxygen equipment, (3) Control breathing [to prevent hyperventilation], (4) Descend [to below 10,000 ft, if possible], and (5) Communicate [with crew/other aircraft/ground].  As hypoxic hypoxia is primarily responsible for disabling conditions and acute symptoms, and it typically clears within 10 seconds of breathing 100% oxygen, the first step is clearly the most important.  Unless being caused by residual toxins in the system, a patient who experienced hypoxia in flight will rarely present with any acute symptoms.  If  acute hypoxia-like symptoms are present post-flight, strongly consider Decompression Sickness (DCS) as a possibility.  DCS will be discussed in detail later in this article.

Hyperventilation

As in any environment, hyperventilation is a possibility during flight.  The mechanism and symptoms are no different in the flight regime than in any other.  It is mentioned here in order to bring attention to several flight-specific potential causes, as well as some concerns regarding the symptoms.

In addition to being induced by anxiety (which can certainly be present in aviation), a feature of the standard military oxygen regulator can induce hyperventilation.  As stated above, 100% oxygen is delivered to aircrew members at altitudes above 10,000 ft.  Because of the physics of gas exchange, at altitudes above 34,000 ft even 100% oxygen, alone, is not enough to maintain sufficient oxyhemoglobin saturation; it must be delivered at an increased pressure.  Because this positive pressure delivery effectively reverses the passive/active phases of the breathing cycle, excessively deep and/or fast breaths may taken - sufficient to initiate a hyperventilation response.  Anecdotally, it has been observed in the altitude chamber that when a person becomes acutely hypoxic (due to altitude hypoxia) that they may start to hyperventilate, presumably as a response to feelings of air hunger.  Frequently if a hypoxic student who is hyperventilating is placed on 100% oxygen the symptoms will initially worsen (called oxygen paradox) before they clear.

Of operational concern is the similarity in symptoms between hyperventilation and acute hypoxia. (Many of the hyperventilation symptoms actually are hypoxia symptoms due to the pH-induced oxyhemoglobin curve shift to the left and the cerebral vasoconstriction resultant from the hypocapnia).  Because of the similarity in the symptom sets, aircrew are instructed to treat both hypoxia and hyperventilation with the same procedure (described above).

Trapped Gas Expansion (TGE)

Boyle's Law states that the volume and pressure of a gas vary inversely, given a constant temperature.  What the means in aviation is that as altitude increases (and pressure decreases), the volume of any gases trapped in cavities of the body will expand.  As the relationship is linear, when the pressure drops by one half (at 18,000 ft), the volume will be approximately double (exactly double, if the trapped gas was "ideal"). The four areas of concern are the middle ear, the sinuses, the GI tract, and the teeth.

The Middle Ear

The middle ear (containing the ossicles) is a small cavity bound with bone everywhere except for the tympanic membrane (eardrum).  Air contained in the middle ear will expand as altitude increases, pressing out against the eardrum, resulting in pain and possible physical damage to the membrane.  The middle ear is also equipped with a small opening leading to the eustachian tube which in turn opens out in the nasopharangeal cavity.  It is the presence of the eustachian tube that allows for "equalizing" pressure changes; as the gas expands, the excess bleeds out through the tube, allowing pressure on both sides of the eardrum to remain equal.  Upon descent, the opposite situation occurs.  As the pressure increases, the gas in the middle ear contracts, causing the eardrum to be "sucked in" towards the middle ear.  Again the eustachian allows for equalization by allowing air to pass back into the middle ear from the nasopharangeal cavity.

In some cases, especially if there is congestion, air may not be able to pass freely through the eustachian tube.  This condition is known as an "ear block", symptoms of which can range from muffled hearing to excruciating pain.  If left untreated, rupture or perforation of the eardrum can be the endpoint. During the ascent, when air must be expelled out of the middle ear, yawning, chewing, or swallowing all may be effective in helping to open the eustachian tube (by physically altering the shape of the opening into the nasopharangeal cavity).  On descent (which is generally when most ear blocks occur), when air must be forced back into the middle ear, the above may also be useful, but the definitive "treatment" is to perform a Valsalva Maneuver.  The procedures for this are: (1) Tip the head back 10 degrees, (2) Pinch off the nose, and (3) With the mouth closed, exhale forcefully.  This effectively forces air back up the eustachian tube by increasing the pressure in the nasopharangeal cavity.  The exhalation should be a short burst vice a lengthy force.  This can also be used as a diagnostic pre-flight procedure; if the ears do not "pop" on the deck, then the individual should not attempt flight that day.  During altitude chamber training, a spray decongestant (such as neosynepherin) is very effectively used to aid in clearing ear blocks.  Although use by aircrew is a violation of the NATOPs self-medication rule, anecdotal evidence indicates that most aircrew carry Afrin with them during flight, preferring to have to explain their actions to the flight surgeon rather than to risk damaging their eardrums.  This is actually done at the advice of most flight surgeons.

Sinuses

For all practical purposes, the problems associated with the sinuses during flight are identical to those associated with the middle ears - except for the location.  All four of the sinuses are potential sites for blocks to occur, however the frontal and maxillary are by far the most common locations.  Like ear blocks, sinus blocks are more likely to occur during descent, and also like ear blocks, the Valsalva maneuver is the primary "treatment" available to aircrew during flight.  All above comments about decongestants apply to sinus conditions as well.

Sinus blocks are more problematic than are ear blocks in two ways: the potential for long-term tissue damage, and inability to check their condition prior to flight.  Even if an eardrum perforates, the healing time is fairly rapid, with very few instances of permanent damage.  A severe sinus block can cause mucous membranes to tear/separate, cartilage to break, and extensive infections.  The grounding time can last months and cases of permanent grounding have been reported.  For this reason, aircrew are generally more concerned about sinus blocks than ear blocks.  The second concern is that there is no definitive way of knowing if sinuses will be a problem during flight.  With ears, a Valsalva maneuver can be performed on the ground.  No such check exists for sinuses.  The bottom line is that any indications of upper respiratory congestion are considered to be incompatible with flying.

Gastrointestinal (GI) Tract

Generally speaking, this is not considered a great threat to flight safety or a great health risk to aircrew, but it is worth mentioning, at least to first-time flyers, as it can, in severe cases produce considerable discomfort.  The presence of gases in the digestive tract is not arcane knowledge.   These gases will respond to Boyle's Law the same way in which will any gas.  The presence of higher levels of water vapor in these gases make them somewhat more responsive than the relatively dry gas found in the ears and sinuses.  Without working through all of the math, an approximate "gouge point" given to aircrew is that at 25,000 ft, any gases in the digestive tract will be approximately three times their sea level volume.

The natural elasticity and release mechanisms of the GI tract minimize the concerns for this expansion, although it can lead to discomfort.  Aircrew are briefed to naturally vent the gas as it expands, and to perform abdominal massage if necessary.  Avoiding typical gas producing foods prior to flight can be preventative.  There are some reports of gas expansion, and the resulting discomfort, so severe that a vaso-vagal response is initiated resulting in tacycardia and syncope.  These reports are anecdotal and quite possibly apocryphal.

Teeth

Gas expansion in the teeth is referred to as barodontalgia, and is very rare.  Poorly performed or broken-down dental work (fillings, crowns, etc) have the potential for leaving small air pockets within the tooth which will respond to Boyle's Law and will expand.  Depending on proximity, the expanding air can cause pressure on the nerves, inducing pain.  Alternatively, if the air pocket is in an external portion of dental reconstructions, fillings can be loosened and actually fall out.  Although there are well documented cases of this, it is very rare.

When the mechanism for this problem is considered, one realizes that it can only occur during the ascent phase of flight, when the gas is expanding.  There are, however, many reported cases of "tooth pain" during descent.  This pain is actually referred pain from a block in the maxillary sinus, and should be treated as described above.

Decompression Sickness (DCS)

Although of greater concern to the undersea arena of operations, DCS is a very real threat to aircrew, given the correct environmental conditions. In the simplest definition, DCS is the formation of gas (primarily Nitrogen) bubbles in the tissues of the body.

Physics and Gas Bubble Formation

The exact mechanisms of gas bubble formation are very complex, and are still not completely understood. It is clear that Henry's Law (the amount of gas that will dissolve in a solution is directly proportional to the pressure above the solution) plays an important role, but when the physics of gas behavior is studied more closely, it is found that additional (not fully identified) factors must be present. 

It is generally believed that there are two requisite conditions for DCS to occur: super-saturation of nitrogen in the tissues - a condition that occurs when the body's natural off-gassing mechanism is not able to "keep up with" pressure changes, usually when there is a 50% decrease in pressure (i.e. exposure to altitudes above 18,000 ft), and the presence of micro-bubble nuclei (seed bubbles).  There is fairly strong empirical data to support the former; there are virtually no reported cases of DCS occurring with exposures of less than 18,000 ft, except for a few unusual cases with major predisposing factors present (which will be discussed later).  The micro-bubble nuclei theory is widely accepted in both aviation and undersea communities and accounts for observed bubble formation at altitudes lower than predicted by application of pure gas law physics.  The most likely genesis of these micro-bubbles lies in naturally hydrophobic segments of some membranes and the varying hydrostatic pressures in synovial fluid during joint movement, although there is still considerable ongoing discussion of this.  Whatever their origin, it is believed that these micro-bubbles cluster and/or grow in size, during the pressure reduction, until bubbles large enough to be symptomatic are formed.  

It should be noted that the discussion of DCS does not involve bubbles forming in the blood.  Although there may be some linkage between Venous Gas Emboli (VGE) and DCS, the DCS symptom sets do not reflect this.  Arterial Gas Embolism (AGE) is a very real and potentially fatal condition that is caused by an overpressurization of the lungs and results in air bubbles traveling through the carotid artery to the brain.  This condition is discussed in more detail in the under sea portions of this CD ROM.

The standard classification of DCS symptoms identifies four manifestations of DCS: the "Bends" (joint pain), Paresthesia (also called "skin bends"), Chokes (pulmonary involvement), and Central Nervous System (sometimes called the "staggers").  Classically, these have been grouped into two "types": Type I - "Non life Threatening" and Type II - "Life Threatening", however current literature is moving away from this classification.  A brief description of each of the 4 manifestations follows.

The Bends

The Bends, or joint pain, is the most common form of DCS, and accounts for 65 to 70% of reported aviation cases.  The bubbles form in the synovial fluid and produce a deep ache.  The large joints are most commonly affected, however any joint with a synovium  is a potential site.  By itself, the Bends is not life-threatening - but as with any suspected DCS case, immediate medical management is required.

Paresthesia

Sometimes called "skin bends", this is the formation of bubbles in the subcutaneous fat layer.  Symptoms are typically a tingling, itching, crawling, or burning sensation in a localized area.  It most commonly shows up on the trunk regions of the body, with some reports of upper arm and thigh occurrences.  This form of DCS is considered the least dangerous and will frequently self-resolve in a matter of minutes.

In some rare occasions, the sensation is accompanied by a red/purple rash.  This condition is known as cutis marmarota, and is considered to be of a much more serious nature as it is indicative of a neurocirculatory effect of bubbles within the system.  Some evidence indicates that 10% of patients presenting with cutis marmarota progress to circulatory collapse if left untreated.

The Chokes

This is the rarest form of DCS in both aviation and diving, accounting for less than 2% of reported cases.  Although rare, it is extremely serious in nature.  The cause of the chokes is bubble formation around the lungs, however a single mechanism for this bubble formation is has not been universally accepted.  Chokes presents as a very specific triptych of symptoms: sub-sternal chest pain, dyspnea, and a dry cough.  If left untreated, collapse is almost inevitable, possibly within minutes.

Central Nervous System (CNS)

CNS DCS is present in 35 to 50% of altitude DCS cases where symptoms do not self-resolve during descent.  All cases of CNS DCS involve bubble formation in the cerebrospinal fluid, either in the spinal column or in the brain.  Statistically, spinal column DCS is very rare in aviation cases, but is quite prevalent in diving cases.  Just the opposite id true of cerebral DCS.  The reason for this dissimilarity is not understood.  Symptoms of spinal column DCS are similar to any cord lesion, with loss of feeling and/or motor control of a limb or limbs being the most common.  Symptoms of cerebral DCS are consistent with a non-specific brain lesion and include, most commonly, dizziness, headache, disorientation, sensory decrements, personality changes, memory loss, and unconsciousness.  In some reported cases, brain stem function has been compromised.  Regardless of how "mild" the symptoms may appear, immediate medical management is essential for patient recovery.  One study indicated that 85% of patients who's symptoms resolved upon descent had recurrence of symptoms post flight.  There are reported cases of permanent neural deficiency following CNS DCS cases.

Of special note for all forms of DCS is the potential for delayed onset of symptoms.  An aircrew member can be completely asymptomatic during the flight, yet have symptoms form hours later.  Questions to rule out DCS should always be included when evaluating an aircrew member.

Predisposing Factors and Prevention

The predisposing factor that is by far the greatest concern is SCUBA diving.  When diving, the tissues are become more highly saturated with nitrogen than they are at a normal sea level equilibrium.  If an individual flys while in this condition, the altitude at which super-saturation occurs will be lower, thus increasing the risk for bubble formation.  There have been reported cases of DCS at altitudes below 10,000 ft following SCUBA diving.  For this reason, NATOPS requires a 24 hour period between SCUBA diving and flying.  As a side note, most civilian sport diver courses also teach this policy; there have been reported DCS cases in commercial jet liners at a cabin altitude of 8,000 ft in individuals who went straight from diving to the airport.  Remember, this is an aircrew regulation; other operational communities may have different requirements.

Other factors that have been found to be "predisposing" in nature are: dehydration, heavy exercise at altitude, joint injuries, and age.  There have long standing beliefs that body composition (body fat), gender, and level of physical fitness are also factors, however there is little empirical data to support these beliefs.

The only real tools for prevention are to avoid as many predisposing factors as possible, and to maintain cabin pressurization.  If flying in tactical jet aircraft, breathing 100% oxygen is also preventative, in that it helps purge some of the nitrogen out of the system, thus reducing the chances for super-saturation.  Thirty minutes of breathing 100% oxygen can reduce the nitrogen levels in the tissues by up to 40%.  For this reason, "pre-breathing" 100% oxygen is used in some altitude chamber training profiles, and may be beneficial in some operational applications.

Treatment

The definitive treatment for all DCS cases is recompression.  Typically a standard Table VI is used, however many variables factor into the decision made by the recompression team.  Occasionally the treatment is limited to breathing 100% while under observation.  When suspected during flight, aircrew are instructed to go to 100% oxygen, descend to as low an altitude as possible, land as quickly as possible, and see the flight surgeon immediately.  Occasionally a pilot will declare an in-flight medical emergency and will have an ambulance waiting at the flight line.  Grounding time following a DCS case is variable and will depend on the nature and severity of the symptoms, as well as the presence of any residual deficiencies.  Thirty days is typical, but the period could be longer.

Sustained Acceleration and G-Induced Loss of Consciousness (GLOC)

Loss of consciousness due to high G-forces is one of the single greatest physiological threats to tactical jet aircrew.  "Pulling Gs" is a vital element of air combat maneuvers; something that all tactical aircrew manage on a regular basis.  Even highly trained and proficient aviators, however, do have occasional problems.  Although this article will discuss theory and basic principles, EVERYONE who is going to fly in this environment MUST have one-on-one training in the proper techniques for maintaining consciousness during high G maneuvers.

G Forces - The Basics

The basic principle of physics that is central to this entire discussion is that of centripetal acceleration, or the vector that pushes from the center of a circle to the outside of a circle during angular motion.  It is the force that holds water in a bucket if swung rapidly over head and the force that causes things to slide across a car seat if a corner is taken quickly.  Because of the aerodynamics of flight, this almost always translates to the vertical axis of the body (pushing towards the feet)  and is called the Gz axis. The effect of this is the feeling of a multiplication of the normal (downward or footward) pull of gravity - regardless of the actual physical orientation of the aircraft.

The unit used in discussing G forces is the "G", which is defined as the normal pull of gravity (32ft/sec2); "2 Gs" would be double that pull, "3 Gs" triple, and so on.  As a general rule of thumb, the "average" human can tolerate up to 3.5 Gs (sustained) without losing consciousness, although some individuals could have a tolerance as high as 5 Gs or as low as 2Gs.  Additionally, an individual's tolerance may change from day to day, depending on a wide variety of factors.  Although there are some predictors (discussed below), the only way to assess tolerance is in a dynamic G environment.

The Physiology of G Forces

In the simplest of terms, maintaining consciousness in any G environment is a function of the blood pressure pushing the blood up to the brain with greater force than the force of gravity pulling blood away from the brain.  The following briefly describes the effects of G forces on major physiological systems.

Cardiovascular System

As increased Gs are experienced, blood will tend to be pulled from the upper portions of the circulatory system to the lower portions resulting in a drop in blood volume above the heart.  Simultaneously, the increased acceleration reduces venous return from the lower extremities, thus reducing cardiac stroke volume.  As this happens, a concurrent drop in blood pressure is measured by the carotid baroreceptors. The baroreceptors trigger a compensatory reflex response which increases the heart rate, partially countering the lowered stroke volume, in a attempt to maintain cardiac output.  Ultimately, the cardiovascular response will not be enough to maintain sufficient cardiac output for retention of consciousness.  Through artificial means of increasing venous return and elevating the blood pressure to the head, consciousness can be maintained.  These methods will be discussed later.

It should be noted that various EKG abnormalities have been observed during centrifuge training/research on individuals exposed to high Gs, however a discussion of those changes is beyond the scope of this article.

Pulmonary Function

The primary effect of elevated Gs on the pulmonary system is in the ventilation-perfusion relationship.  As Gs increase, blood is pulled to the lower portion of the lung, creating a vertical profusion gradient; low perfusion at the top and high perfusion at the bottom.  Due to the relative differences in hydrostatic pressure between blood and air, as the blood settles, the alveoli in the lower portions of the lungs tend to be compressed - some to the point of collapse.  Conversely, as the hydrostatic pressure drops in the upper portions of the lungs, there is room for greater expansion (e.g. ventilation). The result of this is a cross gradient; the area of highest ventilation has the lowest perfusion and the area of lowest ventilation has the highest perfusion.  The end result of this is that approximately 50% of the blood that passes through the lungs does not achieve a gas exchange with the alveoli.  Use of 100% oxygen during high G maneuvers can greatly reduce this problem (lowering the number to approximately 15%), however there are other problems associated with breathing 100% oxygen under high Gs - primarily that of atelectasis.

 Neurological Concerns: Greyout, Blackout, and GLOC

Clearly the greatest threat to the tactical jet aviator when performing high G maneuvers is losing consciousness.  The cause of the loss of consciousness is the brain's inability to function on the amount of blood that the circulatory system is capable of supplying.  Although under certain conditions it is possible to proceed directly from consciousness to unconsciousness, typically actual GLOC is preceded by one or two visual phenomena - greyout and blackout. 

The retina is especially sensitive to oxygen depletion - more so than the brain - and as blood is pulled away from the head, the retina is frequently the first to sense the oxygen deficiency.  Intra-ocular pressure may also be a factor in reducing blood flow to the retinal tissue as the blood pressure to the eye decreases.  During early stages of retinal depletion, the peripheral vision is lost and there may be an overall dimming of the visual image.  This condition is called Greyout, and is considered the first warning sign that GLOC may be immanent.  As the depletion becomes more pronounced, central vision is also lost, resulting in a condition called Blackout.  It should be noted that "blackout" is NOT a loss of consciousness, but rather only a failure of the retina; the aviator will still have his mental and motor faculties available.

Ultimately, if the G levels are maintained, oxygenation will fall below the level required to maintain consciousness and GLOC will occur.  GLOC is a true loss of consciousness episode, with a subsequent period of incapacitation.

Developing an accurate predictive time-line for the progression from one phase to another is not possible given the number of general and idiosyncratic variables.  One generalization, however, can be made.  Due to the oxygen reserves in the brain tissue, a virtually unlimited G level is tolerable for approximately 5 seconds (although this, like most physiological responses, has a range across the population).  Beyond this, time until greyout, blackout, and GLOC becomes a function of Gs pulled and individual tolerance factors.

Incapacitation due to GLOC

Ultimately, incapacitation due to loss of consciousness is the endpoint of excessive, unprotected Gs.  The overall period of incapacitation is usually considered to be two separate phases: absolute incapacitation and relative incapacitation.

Absolute Incapacitation - Absolute Incapacitation (AI) is the period of time when the aircrew member is actually unconscious.  Based on centrifuge studies, the average AI time is 12 seconds, with a range of 9 to 22 seconds.  The primary symptom of this phase is unconsciousness.

Relative Incapacitation - Relative Incapacitation (RI) is that recovery period of time following the regaining of consciousness before a pilot is able regain his situational awareness; i.e. become functional as a pilot.  The period also averages 12 seconds, with a range from 5 to 40 seconds.  Symptoms of this phase are in many ways similar to any period after regaining consciousness; disorientation, confusion, stupor, apathy.  Additional symptoms that may be present are amnesia of the event, convulsive flailing, tingling, euphoria, anxiety, and nausea.  Even after the pilot has fully regained his faculties, there are some symptoms that could persist for the remainder of the flight such as uneasiness, disassociation, loss of will to fight, denial of the event, and depression.

Prevention of GLOC

Through extensive centrifuge-based research, advancements in both hardware and procedures have been made that allow aircrew to operate in a significantly increased G environment.

The Anti-G Suit

The anti-G suit (commonly know simply as the "G-suit") is a pair of cut away trousers, containing 5 interconnected air bladders, that is worn over the flight suit.  The air bladders (located at the abdomen, thighs, and calves) are connected to a valve (the "G valve") on the aircraft via a hose.  When the G-valve senses an increase in Gs on the aircraft, it automatically opens, forcing air into the bladders, which in turn expand.  The expansion of the bladders compresses the tissues of the lower body, reducing the amount of blood that can pool in the legs thus increasing both cardiac output and upper torso blood pressure.  For obvious reasons, to function properly the G suit must be snuggly fit, and requires custom sizing for each aircrew.  When properly fit the G-suit will increase tolerance by approximately 1 to 1 1/2 Gs. 

Researchers are constantly looking for ways to make the G-suit more effective.  The most recent advance is called Navy Combat Edge (NCE).  NCE combines a larger coverage G-suit, positive pressure breathing, and a chest counter-pressure vest, all linked to the G-valve.  The purpose of the NCE is not to necessarily increase the maximum G attainable (although this may occur), but rather reduce the fatigue level of the AGSM (see below) and increase endurance.  A full technical discussion of the equipment exceeds the scope of this article, but preliminary reports from aircrew indicate that "it makes 7 Gs feel like 5".

The Anti-G Straining Maneuver (AGSM)

The AGSM is central to all GLOC prevention strategies and is one of the most important skills that  tactical jet aircrew must master.  Although "AGSM" is the generally accepted "generic" term, historically this  procedure has been known as the M-1, L-1, Grunting, Groaning, and currently "Hook" maneuver (which will be used for the remainder of this article).  Interestingly, throughout all of these name changes the technique has remained virtually unchanged. There are two basic components to the Hook maneuver: isometric muscle contraction and forced exhalation.

Although the G-suit helps to reduce blood pooling in the legs, it has been found that hard isometric contraction of the lower abdomen and leg muscles greatly increases the benefits of the G-suit.  The recommended application of this component of the Hook maneuver is to initiate contraction prior to the onset of Gs, and to hold it constantly until the release of the Gs, regardless of the fluctuation of the actual Gs experienced.

The forced exhalation component of the maneuver is where the name "hook" originates.  Technically, this breathing is described as "a forced exhalation against a closed glottis".  As that can leave aircrew confused, it was found that if the word "hook" said, without the "k" (kind of a clipped "huh"), the glottis closes appropriately.  When this is performed with a very hard diaphragmatic contraction, the inter-thoracic chest pressure is increased which in turn increases the blood pressure to the head - allowing for perfusion during increased Gs.  The same increase in chest pressure, however, also impairs venous blood return to the heart and can result in a compromise to the circulatory system.  For this reason, unlike the isometric contractions of the legs which may be continued indefinitely (limited only by muscle endurance), the chest pressure must be released, very briefly, on a periodic basis to allow for venous return.  To perform this release, the "k" from the "hook" is sounded and a small, partial, air exchange is accomplished, followed immediately by the next "huh".  It must be understood that this is NOT a true breath, but rather a brief release in pressure, ideally not lasting longer than 1 second.  Extensive research has been done on this issue, and repeated studies all point to 3 to 5 seconds as the ideal length of time to hold the "huh" before adding the "k".  If held longer than this, venous return becomes a problem; if done more frequently, fatigue becomes a significant factor (when done at high G levels, this is extremely tiring due to the number of muscles being used and the contraction force required).  The literature is somewhat conflicted regarding the quantifiable benefits gained from the Hook maneuver, but 3 to 4 additional Gs of protection seems to be typical.

The two components work together in the following way.  As soon as increased Gs are felt (ideally a few seconds prior to the onset) the muscle contraction component should be performed.  This should accomplished to the maximum level obtainable by the muscles.  Once begun, it should not be released until all increased Gs are released.  IMMEDIATELY following the contraction, the first "Hook" should be performed, also to the highest level obtainable by the muscles.  Once the G loading is determined, the amount of force used on the Hook can be varied as needed to prevent greyout from occurring.  The breathing component is repeated on a 3 to 5 second interval until all increased Gs are released.  Given a correctly fit G-suit, a properly performed Hook maneuver, and practice, aircrew can reach 9Gs for 30 seconds (in a centrifuge) without blacking out.

Factors Effecting G Tolerance

There are numerous factors that can all effect an individual's G tolerance level.  Knowledge of these can also help aircrew avoid GLOC.  The following list is representative, but should not be considered all-inclusive.  One must keep in mind that "tolerance" is actually a two dimensional measure, incorporating both maximum G level and exposure time.

G Level - The higher the Gs experienced, the lower the tolerance and the shorter the time until GLOC.  As stated earlier, the basic G tolerance level for the unprotected, untrained human (referred to as the resting tolerance) is approximately 3.5 Gs.  Anecdotally, many experienced tactical jet aircrew have a resting tolerance between 4.5 and 5.0 Gs.

Duration of Exposure - The longer the exposure, the greater the chances of experiencing difficulties.  Beyond the 5 second oxygen reserve, there are no satisfactory tools for predicting actual individual tolerance times.

Dehydration - Research has demonstrated that dehydration will lower G tolerance.

Experience - The Hook maneuver is a learned skill that takes practice. It naturally follows that more experience will lead to better proficiency.  There is are also anecdotal  evidence that when exposed to a high G environment on a regular basis that the body's natural tolerance increases.

Currency of Experience - Although the mechanism is not understood, aircrew almost universally report that their tolerance decreases if they have been in a non flying status for a period of time (as short as a few days). This may be the result of less effective Hook maneuvers ("use it or lose it") and/or some unidentified physiological accommodation.  "Warm up" turns are typically completed by aircrew prior to all actual high-G missions, which help them gauge their G tolerance for that day.

Illness - Acute illness will result in a lowered physical ability which will translate directly to lowered performance on the Hook maneuver.  Extended illness will remove the aircrew from the aircraft for days/weeks, and result in the problems discussed above.

Physical Conditioning - Due to the physical demands of the hook maneuver, weight training is considered mandatory for any aircrew who regularly operate in the high G environment.  Both strength and endurance training is required for maximal performance.  In rough terms, strength training will increase maximum G levels and endurance training will increase duration of exposure times.  Heavy exercise just prior to flying is not encouraged, and the importance of re-hydrating must be stressed.

Blood Pressure - Aircrew with low blood pressures may have a lower G tolerance than those with higher blood pressures.  Aircrew are NOT encouraged to pursue a lifestyle that will increase their blood pressures, however they are cautioned that extreme aerobic fitness programs could have a negative effect on G tolerance.  Studies have shown that an aggressive weight training program can "counter" the effects of an aggressive aerobic training program.

Spatial Disorientation

Spatial Disorientation (SD) is frequently cited as the single greatest threat to aircrew, across the operational platforms. Defined as incorrectly perceiving the position of the aircraft, relative to the earth's surface, SD can range from a simple false sensation of motion to controlled flight into the terrain (CFIT).  This article will first look at basic sensory system function (and dysfunction), some sensory illusions that are common to the flight environment, a classification of SD, and guidelines for the prevention of and recovery from SD.

Sensory Systems

It should probably come as no surprise that the visual system provides the most important cues for orientation; some sources say as much as 80% of all orientation cues come from the eyes.  The vestibular system (semicircular canals and otolith organs) provides approximately 15% of the cues, and the proprioceptive system supplies the remaining 5%.

The Visual System

It is not the purpose of this article to provide information about the basic functional anatomy and physiology of the eye; that information can be found in any standard text.  There are a few terms used in the arena of aerospace physiology that are not common, so they will be addressed here.

Focal Vision - Anatomically, this is the central or foveal vision.  Compromising only 2 - 4% of the total field of view (FOV), this small segment of the retina is responsible for all of the detailed vision and is what is measured during visual acuity tests.  From the perspective of retinal micro-anatomy, this mode of vision uses almost exclusively cone cells.  Cognitively speaking - and this is most important concept of focal vision - the highly refined images fed to the brain from this mode require processing at the conscious level.  This means that this sensory input must be thought about, and compared to previously learned information for it to become usable.  Most notably, this mode of vision is used for all instrument flying, as well as other important in-flight tasks such as target acquisition, distance/altitude estimation, and landing.  The benefit of information coming from the focal vision is its reliability, but the drawbacks are in the narrowness of the field of vision, and the requirement for conscious tasking to interpret the data.

Ambient Vision - Anatomically, this is everything other than the central, foveal vision, and is responsible many basic orientation and motion cues.  Unlike the focal vision which uses exclusively cone cells, the ambient vision uses both cone and rod cells, with an emphasis on the rod cells as the visual field progresses further to the periphery.  Cognitively speaking, the ambient vision is not dependent on conscious processing; most of the information from this mode is processed at the pre or subconscious level. This is a two-edge sword.  On the plus side, this allows for a great volume of basic orientation and motion information to be processed by the brain without having to use "scarce" conscious processing resources, but on the negative side the ambient vision is very prone to experiencing visual illusions.  The sense of motion one can feel when watching a wide-format film or when a car next to you starts rolling backwards are two simple examples.

Under normal circumstances, the two visual modes are operating simultaneously; the ambient flooding the brain with basic "gut-level" orientation cues, and the focal filling in the details as it scans across the visual field.  If the ambient vision is compromised (i.e. flying through heavy clouds or fog), all of the orientation cues must come from the focal vision (i.e. instrument scan) which results in both slower processing and a greater chance for task saturation.  If the focal vision is compromised (i.e. breakdown of instrument scan) then all of the orientation cues will come from the ambient vision, which, due to the number of illusions possible, could lead to disorientation.  Ultimately, both modes of vision, working together, are required to maintain maximal orientation.

The Vestibular System

The vestibular system is a series of primitive sensory organs, primarily "designed" for a low acceleration, two dimensional world.  That they work at all in the highly dynamic flight environment is amazing; it should come as no surprise that they are very prone to illusions.  As with the ambient vision, cognitive processing is done at the pre- or subconscious level.

Semicircular Canals - The semicircular canals detect angular accelerations and decelerations based on the relative motion of fluid in the canals and the canal walls.  As a rotational movement is started, inertia causes the fluid in the canal to lag behind which stimulates hair cells mounted to the side of the canal walls.  During a constant-rate turn, the fluid eventually "catches up" with the canals, and no more motion cues are sent to the brain (even though the rotation has not stopped).  When the rotation is stopped (especially suddenly), inertia causes the fluid to continue move, again stimulating the hairs, creating a false sense of rotation in the direction opposite to the original turn.  The technical term for this is a "somatogyral illusion"

Another function of the semicircular canals is to guide the tracking of the eyes during head movements.  A neural connection between the semicircular canals and the ocular muscles directs eye tracking as a function of semicircular canal stimulation (the vestibulo-ocular reflex).  This type of eye movement is known as nystagmus, and is a "normal" physiological  response to head motion.  When semicircular canals send false information to the orientation centers of the brain, they also send false information to the eyes, the result being the appearance that the visual field is in motion (when it is not).  This is sometimes termed the "oculogyral illusion"

Otolith Organs -  The otolith organs are basically "tilt sensors".  Small calcium carbonate crystals (statoconia) imbedded in a mucosal mass rest on a bed of hairs.  When the head is tilted, gravity moves the crystals across the hairs which in turn send "tilt" information to the brain.  The inertia of the crystals, associated with linear accelerations or decelerations can also cause movement across the bed of hairs resulting in " false tilt" cues being sent to the brain; forward accelerations being perceived as a "nose up" tilt and a forward deceleration being perceived as a "nose down" tilt.  This is termed a "somatogravic illusion".

The Proprioceptive System

This is frequently referred to as the "seat of the pants" sense, and being the most primitive of all of the orientation systems it provides the least amount of useful information for maintaining spatial orientation.  Working on the pre- or subconscious level, this "system" is actually the collection of the various proprioceptors of the body that, through pressure or stretch, give basic positional cues to the brain.

For spatial orientation, the basic cues deal with pressure and the assumed pull of normal gravity; when seated, if pressure is felt on the buttocks it indicates being vertical, if felt on the back it indicate being horizontal, etc.

As with otolith function, linear accelerations can produce false cues.  A forward acceleration will increase pressure on the back creating a false sensation of being nose up.  The converse is true for decelerations.  It should be noted that these false perceptions of pitch reinforce the false sensations that may come from the otolith organs.

Sensory Illusions

Sensory illusions are simply misperceptions based either on confusing sensory information or the malfunctioning of a sensory system.  The study of sensory illusions is, to a large extent, the study of classification and example.  Although dozens of illusions have been named and categorized, they all come down to the basic physiology discussed above.  The following provides a brief discussion of some of the most important of the illusions.

Visual Illusions

Shape/Size Constancy Illusions - Illusions based on assumed size or shape of an observed object on the ground.  The most common examples of this involve runways of an unexpected size.  If flying over a runway that is smaller than "normal", it "looks" smaller, and the natural assumption is that it is farther away (false perception of being too high).  The "natural" correction for this is to reduce altitude - which could place the aircraft at a dangerously low altitude.  The converse is true of larger than "normal" runways.  Similar problems can occur when judging altitude based on size of ground objects (i.e. trees and roads) when the actual size of the objects is not known.  Prevention of this illusion can be accomplished with a thorough brief of runway and terrain features prior to flight.

Absent Focal Cues - This is not so much an illusion as it is a situation of sensory deprivation.  Because an experienced pilot does frequently use ground references to judge altitude, when flying over featureless terrain there is a significant reduction in visual cues available for use.  Examples of this are sand, snow, and smooth water.

Absent Ambient Cues - As a large percent of the subconscious orientation cues derive from the ambient vision (most importantly, the perception of the horizon) if ambient cues are not available, the pilot must depend strictly on focal cues - primarily instruments.  This significantly increases the task loading of the pilot.  Examples of this are flying at night and through fog or heavy clouds.  The "Black Hole" illusion or approach - coming in for a landing at night with no external visual cues but the runway lights - has been cited as the cause of several mishaps.  "White-out" and "Brown-out" are two examples specific to the rotary wing and Harrier communities.  In both of these, vertical landings can create a thick cloud of blowing snow or sand, suddenly obscuring virtually all outside visual cues.

Autokinesis - This is a visual illusion resulting from the normal saccadic movement of the eyes.  Under daylight conditions, the presence of many visual cues obscures these small, subconscious eye movements.  At night time however, especially if there is a single, small point light source (a light on top of a tower, for example), the saccadic movement of the eyes can create the illusion that the light source is actually moving.  There are anecdotal reports of pilots "joining up" on ground structures thinking that they were actually other aircraft.  Conscious scanning of the eyes (vice staring at the source) usually reduces this illusion.

Vection Illusions - Collectively, vection illusions are a false perception of motion resulting from observing another object moving (usually in the peripheral vision).  The most common example of a "linear vection" illusion is the sense of moving forward when a car parked next to rolls backwards.  The perception of motion created in full visual flight simulators and even wide format films also falls into this category.  An example of an "angular vection" illusion can be found in various amusement park rides where a stationary walkway or track goes through the middle of a rotating barrel, creating a strong illusion of rolling or falling over.

False Horizons - Of all the illusions so far discussed, this is probably the most frequent to occur.  The eye has a natural tendency to pick up a strong horizontal line and treat it as a horizon.  In the case of the real horizon, this is an extremely beneficial tool in maintaining orientation, so much so that there are systems currently under development that will project a horizon on the inside of a cockpit, providing this subconscious level of orientation input even at night.  Unfortunately, this same strong cueing can result in a very powerful illusion if the observed line is NOT the true horizon.  The most common example of this is flying over a sloping cloud deck, although ridge lines, coast lines, and at night isolated (well lit) highways have all be cited as sources for this illusion.  As with most cases, cross-checking the instruments is the method of prevention and recovery from this illusion. 

Vestibular Illusions

The "Leans" - The leans is probably the single most common of all of the illusions discussed and is the primary example of a somatogyral illusion.  This illusion is the result of misperceived semicircular canal cues, as described above.  While in a constant rate turn, the inertia-based stimulation ceases, and the sense of turn (bank) is no longer perceived.  When the aircraft is rolled out of the turn, the continued movement of the semicircular canal fluid creates the sensation of rolling into a turn in the opposite direction.  Because this "feels" wrong, the pilot will tend to lean over in the seat to "correct" for the feeling - thus the name.  Alternately, the pilot may re-enter the turn to make it "feel" right.  The oculogyral illusion, as discussed above, can accompany these illusions. Other somatogyral illusions include the "Graveyard Spin" and "Graveyard Spiral", which are both exaggerated forms of the above and are very rare in modern aviation.  Use of instruments will help the pilot recover from any of these illusions

Coriolis Cross-Coupling - This is an illusion of spinning or tumbling that results from conflicting stimulation of the semi-circular canals.  The most common cause of this illusion is the movement of the head during turning or spinning motions.  When this occurs, the semi-circular canals have to simultaneously respond to an angular  deceleration in one plane and an angular acceleration in another plane.  The resultant effect can be one of turning or tumbling in an entirely different plane than either of the two actually being experienced.

Inversion Illusion - This is a very serious example of a somatogravic illusion (see above), and can occur during a level off following a high performance climb by a tactical jet aircraft, typically at night or in the clouds.  As a pilot noses the aircraft over to level flight, two acceleration vectors come into play: a negative G vector pulling the pilot up from the seat, and a forward acceleration vector pushing the pilot backwards.  Without visual references, the net "feeling" of this is rolling backwards out of the seat - what would happen if the aircraft was steeply pitched up, starting to enter a loop.  Following the "natural" tendency to push the nose down will only intensify the effect of the two acceleration vectors, and will put the aircraft into a dive.  If not corrected (by flying the instruments) this clearly could, and has, led to ground impact.

G-Excess Illusion - This is an unusual illusion in that it is an otolith organ based illusion that involves turning maneuvers.  While in a turning maneuver, if the pilot looks to the inside of the turn, the centripetal acceleration will cause the statoconia of the otoliths to move back across the hair bed, creating a "pitch up" sensation.  While looking to the inside of the turn, this will result in the feel of a reduction in the angle of bank.  A pilot will "correct" for this by increasing the angle of bank, which will result in a loss of lift and altitude.  If the pilot turns his head forward, the resulting illusion will be of excessive nose up pitch, which the pilot will "correct" by lowering the nose, again losing altitude.

Mechanisms and Classifications of Spatial Disorientation

It should be clear by now that there is a wide variety of potential causes for spatial disorientation, and a wide range of effects - from simple experiences of "vertigo" to incapacitating loss of control to controlled flight into the terrain.  What, then, is the primary cause of disorientation?  If not a "cause", a common thread throughout all disorientation situations is a breakdown in focal vision inputs to the conscious processing centers of the brain; more simply  put, a breakdown in the instrument scan - typically caused by either complacency or task overloading.  Without the dependable information from the instruments, the brain must rely on the subconscious inputs from the ambient vision, vestibular, and proprioceptive systems.  As long as there are no illusions present, spatial orientation will likely be maintained, but if an illusion does occur during this period of scan break down, then spatial disorientation, to some extent, should be expected.

Classifications of Spatial Disorientation

Type I - Unrecognized Disorientation - Virtually all SD starts as unrecognized.  As indicated above, a breakdown in the scan occurs, and an illusion is present.  For the duration of the scan break down, as long as the illusion is present and the pilot flies the aircraft based on the false perceptions of the illusion, unrecognized SD is being experienced.  Type I SD is generally considered to be the most dangerous, simply because the pilot does not even realize that anything is wrong.  It is believed that many CFIT mishaps are the direct result of unrecognized SD.

Type II - Recognized Disorientation - If Type I SD is the start of all SD occurrences, then Type II could be considered the first step in all successful SD recoveries.  Using the broadest definition of Type II SD, it occurs anytime that there is a mismatch in sensory cues.  This would clearly describe the situation following the re-establishment of an instrument scan and discovering that the aircraft was not doing what the pilot thought it was.  Another example would be that of a pilot who anticipated an illusion before executing a maneuver; things may "feel" wrong, but by remaining on the instruments correct control of the aircraft can be maintained.  The not entirely understood concept of visual (focal) dominance is credited for the brain's ability to use focal vision inputs to override ambient, vestibular, and proprioceptive inputs.

Type III - Incapacitating Disorientation - The least understood of the three types, this is generally described as a breakdown in the communication channels between the conscious and subconscious portions of the brain, resulting in the pilot not being able to discern which sensory cues are accurate and which are false. Not able to mentally process the mismatched sensory information, the pilot becomes incapacitated - at least in regards to his ability to correctly control the aircraft.  Unresolved, this type of SD will almost always result in a mishap (crash).  Some anecdotal studies indicate that this is primarily a concern in the tactical jet community, although this may be due to the presence of ejection seats, which will allow an extremely disoriented jet pilot an escape option not available to other aircrew types.

Spatial Disorientation Prevention and Recovery

Prevention

The most basic prevention technique is to maintain a solid instrument scan, and if an discrepancy arises between the instruments and any other sensory information, follow what the instruments say - unless there is a very real reason to believe that there may be a mechanical malfunction in the instruments.  Reducing task loading can help prevent scan breakdown from occurring; various techniques of compartmentalization can be used to this end.  To help reduce the incidence of illusions, a thorough pre-flight brief of geographical features should be conducted.

Recovery

The bottom line to all SD is this:  Fly the instruments!  As soon as the first hint of SD is suspected, the pilot must get on the instruments and stay on them until the SD has been resolved.  If the disoriented pilot is in a multi-pilot aircraft, transferal of control to the less disoriented pilot is recommended.  Identifying the illusion or cause of sensory mismatch can also help the pilot focus on the instruments.  If a semicircular canal or otolith organ illusion is suspected, minimizing head movements will help the illusion resolve itself.   Finally, if the SD cannot be resolved and an incapacitating situation has developed, the pilot should egress (eject) from the aircraft while/if it is possible.

Loss of Situational Awareness

Loss of Situational Awareness (LSA) is a very commonly used, if not well defined term in virtually all discussions of aircrew human factors. Some sources equate LSA with spatial disorientation, but in actuality, it is a much broader concept than that.  Spatial disorientation, collectively, is a subset of LSA - but there are many other ways in which a pilot can lose his over all situational awareness, yet still be spatially oriented (i.e. he can be in perfect straight and level flight, and know it, but be 50 miles off course).

The lack of a single definition can sometimes lead to confusion as to what exactly is meant by the term.  In it's most basic form, LSA is simply "losing track" of some aspect of the mission.  In more technical terms, it can be viewed as a failure in attention, most frequently - although not exclusively - due to task saturation.  To better appreciate this, a brief discussion of cognitive task management is appropriate.

Multiple Task Management

Obviously, a human can perform multiple tasks simultaneously - whether it's as basic as "walking and chewing gum" or driving down a busy interstate, following a map, listening to the radio, and talking with a passenger.  In order to accomplish multiple tasks, we have developed several strategies - overlearning, integration, and task shifting - to aid in the process.  Central to all of these is that some tasks require the focus of our conscious attention to perform, and some only require marginal attention. Each type will be discussed briefly below.

Overlearning

Associated primarily with motor (vice cognitive) tasks, overlearning is the process by which a task is learned so well, that it does not require conscious thought to perform and can function in the margin.  A common example of this is basic driving skills.  At first every aspect of driving must be concentrated on, but with practice, an experience driver does not have to consciously think about every adjustment of the steering wheel, accelerator, etc.  The parallels to basic airwork involved with flying are obvious.  Every task that is moved to the margin, is one less that requires the limited resources of attention focus.

Integrating

Also primarily associated with motor functions, integration is the process of combining a group of related tasks into a single task.  Continuing with the driving example, manually shifting gears serves as a good illustration.  Clearly, there are three separate tasks involved (clutch, shift, and accelerator), and in the learning process each task must be concentrated on (as well as the coordination between the three).  As the learning process progresses, the three tasks blend into a single task (shifting gears).  In this particular example, the integrated task may then become overlearned, the net result being three attention-focused tasks becoming a single attention margin task.

Task Shifting

Sometimes called "task swapping", this is the primary means available for multiple cognitive task processing. As the name implies, the mechanism for this process is the continuous shifting of the conscious attention from one task to another, in a cyclical pattern. A basic assumption made is that each task can function without constant attention, as long as it is tended to periodically.  Clearly, a pilot's instrument scan is the most important example of this in aviation.  Also, each task will require a different amount of time to be attended to appropriately; some may require only a second or two, 3 or 4 times a minute, others may require 15 or 20 second time blocks.  It must be remembered that there are some tasks that require full attention for their duration, and are not compatible with shifting.

Attention Span and Task Saturation

Using a combination of the above techniques, multiple tasks can be effectively accomplished.  "Attention Span", in the context of this discussion, indicates the maximum number of tasks that an individual can effectively handle at a given time.  Frequently, the number of required tasks will fall below this level.  When the number of tasks required of an individual equals this number, then the individual is said to be "Task Saturated" and the individual can only assimilate a new task by dropping an existing one.  The reality of the flight environment is a dynamic, constantly changing series of tasks, with new ones being added and old ones dropped, based on the prioritization of a given task at a given time.

Prioritization, Misprioritization, and Channelization

A human factors sage once said "Task saturation has never killed anyone, task misprioritization has killed many".  Whether completely true or not, this statement does bring into focus the final aspect of situational awareness - task prioritization.  Any time that multiple tasks are being performed, there is a hierarchy of importance ranging from absolutely essential to optional.  Every time a new task is presented, the hierarchy must be reviewed and possibly changed, depending on the nature of the new task.  These concepts are best understood by example.

Going back to a driving example, the following prioritization hierarchy may be present:  (1) Control the car, (2) Obey the law, (3) Follow a map, (4) Talk with a passenger, and (5) Listen to the radio.  For the sake of this example, assume that this represents even task saturation; all of these tasks can be performed efficiently, no more can be added.  Now, the driver's cell phone rings, and he is presented with a choice; either ignore the new task and maintain the currently functioning task load, or answer the phone at the expense of one of the existing tasks.  If he chooses the latter, then a second decision must be made: which of the existing tasks will be set aside in order to respond to the new one?  Correct prioritization will start on the bottom of the hierarchy and work up.  Once the nature of the call is ascertained, then a reevaluation of the tasking hierarchy must be made, as well as an informed decision regarding which task (s) must be eliminated.

As long as the mission essential tasks remain at the top of the list, and the tasks of lesser importance remain at the bottom, then correct prioritization is maintained, and the maximal level of situational awareness allowed by the situation is maintained.  In the above example, the driver can turn off the radio, stop talking to his friend, and answer the phone, then decide which of these tasks is the more important and proceed from there.  The problem occurs when ALL of the existing tasks are mission essential, AND the new one is also.  When this occurs, some aspect of situational awareness will be lost, the important decision being which element is the least important.

Returning to the example, assume that the passenger is an important client with whom the driver is trying to close a major contract, and that he is listening to traffic reports because there has been an accident and he has to get his client to an important meeting on time, and he needs the map to find the location of the meeting.  The phone call is from his financial officer telling him that the Board of Directors is NOT going to approve the 10% increase in advertising that the client is requesting, threatening the whole contract... From the aspect of driving safety, as long as (1), and to a slightly lesser extent (2) are maintained, there will probably not be an accident (even though the overall outcome of his mission may be effected).  If, however the driver misprioritizes and loses control of the car (or commits gross violations of the law), an accident is a likely outcome.

Channelization occurs when a single task - either existing or new - becomes so important that it eclipses virtually all other tasks.  If this task is, in fact, of crucial importance, channelization on it may be the correct prioritization decision.  If, however, it is a task of lesser importance, then channelization becomes the worst kind of misprioritization and almost inevitably leads to an accident.

Visiting our example one final time let's look at two different illustrations of  channelization, one correct, one incorrect:  (1) The traffic suddenly becomes very heavy at a construction site with multiple  lane merges and detours.  The driver correctly fixates on the traffic situation (at the expense of his client and contract), safely negotiates the construction zone, then returns to his client. (2) The phone call is from the local hospital with news that a family member has been involved in a serious accident and may not live.  The driver becomes so fixated on the phone call that he misses the construction warning signs and causes an accident himself.

Although the above has centered on a driving example, it should be fairly clear how this could just as easily been a flying example (flying the aircraft, obeying flight rules, following a chart, maintaining crew coordination with his navigator, and listening to the radio for calls from the controllers).

Given all of the above, LSA can most simply, and accurately, be described as the result of misprioritization during a period of task saturation.

One additional factor should be considered in a discussion of LSA, and that is complacency.  This is especially dangerous on a "routine" mission or on one where there is very low task loading.  At these times, task shifting (scanning) discipline may decrease, and although not due to overloading, some tasks may slip out of appropriate attention focus.  This situation is amplified if an unexpected event occurs (i.e. aircraft emergency) during this time of lowered scanning discipline, because it requires a sudden prioritization of existing tasks coupled with the integration of the new task.   The shift from a complacent low-discipline attitude to an attention-intensive multiple decision making one can be very difficult and may require more time than is available to correctly assess and prioritize all tasks.


This section was contributed by LCDR Brian D. Swan, MSC, USN

*Source: Operational Medicine 2001,  Health Care in Military Settings, NAVMED P-5139, May 1, 2001, Bureau of Medicine and Surgery, Department of the Navy, 2300 E Street NW, Washington, D.C., 20372-5300


 

 

 

Military Obstetrics & Gynecology

This information is provided by The Brookside Associates Medical Education Division.  The Brookside Associates, Ltd. is a private organization, not affiliated with any governmental agency. The opinions presented here are those of the author and do not necessarily represent the opinions of the Brookside Associates or the Department of Defense. The presence of any advertising on these pages does not constitute an endorsement of that product or service by either the US Department of Defense or the Brookside Associates. All material presented here is unclassified.

C. 2009 All Rights Reserved

Contact Us  ·  Other Brookside Products

Military OB-GYN
Contents

Advertise on this site